EEG Analytics for Early Detection of Autism Spectrum Disorder: A data-driven approach

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a complex and heterogeneous disorder, diagnosed on the basis of behavioral symptoms during the second year of life or later. Finding scalable biomarkers for early detection is challenging because of the variability in presentation of the disorder and the need for simple measurements that could be implemented routinely during well-baby checkups. EEG is a relatively easy-to-use, low cost brain measurement tool that is being increasingly explored as a potential clinical tool for monitoring atypical brain development. EEG measurements were collected from 99 infants with an older sibling diagnosed with ASD, and 89 low risk controls, beginning at 3 months of age and continuing until 36 months of age. Nonlinear features were computed from EEG signals and used as input to statistical learning methods. Prediction of the clinical diagnostic outcome of ASD or not ASD was highly accurate when using EEG measurements from as early as 3 months of age. Specificity, sensitivity and PPV were high, exceeding 95% at some ages. Prediction of ADOS calibrated severity scores for all infants in the study using only EEG data taken as early as 3 months of age was strongly correlated with the actual measured scores. This suggests that useful digital biomarkers might be extracted from EEG measurements.

Similar content being viewed by others

Longitudinal EEG power in the first postnatal year differentiates autism outcomes

Article Open access 13 September 2019

Enhancing autism spectrum disorder classification in children through the integration of traditional statistics and classical machine learning techniques in EEG analysis

Article Open access 08 December 2023

Neuroimaging genetics approaches to identify new biomarkers for the early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder

Article Open access 17 April 2023

Introduction

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is defined by a heterogeneous constellation of behavioral symptoms that emerge over the first years of life. A recent Centers for Disease Control (CDC) report estimates that the prevalence of ASD in the United States is 1 in 68, a significant increase in the past decade 1 . As research on ASD continues at rapid pace, the etiology and developmental course appear to be increasingly diverse, resulting in a view of ASD with diverse cognitive, behavioral and neural trajectories and subtypes 2 .

High-risk infant siblings studies have demonstrated that the defining behavioral features of ASD emerge during the latter part of the first and second years of life 3 . Because ASD is behaviorally and not biologically defined, a formal diagnosis of ASD before three years of age remains challenging 4,5 and children often do not receive a diagnosis until the preschool years or later 6 . Milder forms of ASD are particularly difficult to detect early, in part because a broad range of neurodevelopmental symptoms are common to several diagnoses, including ASD 7 . Even in cases of diagnostic uncertainty, reliable biomarkers that detect emerging ASD symptoms might be useful for developing appropriate early interventions 4 .

The brain develops rapidly during the first years of life and atypical neurodevelopment is likely due to a combination of genetics, biological, and environmental conditions, all compounded by adaptations that result from atypical interactions between the developing child and his or her environment 8 . An emerging view is that the behavioral symptoms that define ASD may be the end result of early brain adaptation, rather than the direct consequence of ongoing neural pathology 9 . This view suggests the possibility that the primary neural impairments that lead to ASD are transitory and thus difficult to detect after a critical developmental period 8 .

Neural Correlates of Behavior

Because atypical brain development that leads to ASD symptoms is likely to precede atypical behavior by months or even years, a critical developmental window for early intervention may be missed if diagnosis or screening is based solely on behavioral features. This has fueled a search for early neural correlates or biological indicators that could identify ASD in the prodromal phase.

Some models of ASD are based on atypical development of neural connectivity 10,11,12 , with excessive local connectivity within neural assemblies and deficits in long-range connectivity between functional brain regions implicated. Recent studies suggest the involvement of somatosensory, default mode, visual, and subcortical networks 13 . Microstructural properties of the uncinate fasciculus in 6-month-old infants were found to predict behavioral response to joint attention three months later, indicating the potential importance of this tract in social development 14 . Further, microstructural organization of the splenium of the corpus callosum was associated with atypical oculomotor function in 7-month old infants who later developed ASD 15 . These studies suggest that early atypical neural function, due to atypical neural structure, may be associated with the behavioral symptoms of ASD that appear later. The challenge from a clinical perspective is how to measure atypical neural function for use in monitoring neurodevelopment.

A recent fMRI study of 59 6-month-old infants demonstrated significant differences in the brains of children who would develop a diagnosis of ASD at 24 months of age 12 . While these results are scientifically promising, two significant challenges remain before biomarker measurement methods can be developed for clinical use. First, to be viable in a primary care setting, any brain measurement method should be low cost and simple to administer in the context of a well-baby checkup. Second, ASD is a spectrum disorder that exhibits a heterogeneous set of defining behavioral characteristics. A simple binary determination of ‘autism’ or ‘not-autism’ may not be adequate for broad clinical application, such as monitoring changing risk profiles as a child develops or response to therapeutic interventions.

EEG for Functional Brain Measurement

Our previous studies have attempted to use traditional spectral power analysis to find early biomarkers for autism 16 . While differences were found between high risk siblings and low risk controls, they were not correlated with outcome. A recent study using data from the same infants as in the present study found that reduced frontal high-alpha power at 3 months was associated with lower expressive language skills at 12 months, but did not predict ASD-specific outcomes. Notably, the association between 3-month frontal power and expressive language skills appeared to be developmentally time-locked to the 12-month time point, with no evidence of this association persisting over the second and third years of life in our sample 17 .

The potential for EEG to be used as a functional brain imaging modality continues to improve as new methods for analyzing and extracting information from biophysical signals are developed. Methods for analyzing complex time series produced by complex networks, such as the brain, may enable network structural differences to be inferred from time series measurements 18 , as illustrated in Fig. 1. Based on this, a set of nonlinear or complex system parameters or ‘invariant measures’ from EEG signals in principle should characterize the neural dynamics of the brain that generates the signals.

figure 1

The implication is that the complex time series obtained from EEG contains information about neural network structure that may provide valuable clues about atypical function 19,20 . This has clinical implications because a new generation of low cost, easy to use EEG devices are becoming available 21,22 , which would make routine brain measurements a possibility in a primary care setting.

The challenge for developmental clinical neuroscience is to determine which combinations of invariant measures and scalp locations are most relevant to the detection and monitoring of characteristics relevant to ASD. A data-driven approach may be sufficient for identifying useful clinical biomarkers. Data-driven discovery may also point in the direction of the likely neural correlates of relevant behavioral constructs or cognitive phenotypes 19 . The goal of this study is to demonstrate that nonlinear analysis of EEG signals, together with pattern classification methods, can extract information as early as 3 months of age that predicts an infant will develop a clinical diagnosis of ASD. Furthermore, the severity of ASD symptoms, as measured by the Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale (ADOS), can also be predicted from EEG data taken as early as 3 months of age, with strong correlation to the real ADOS scores that the child has at three years of age. Our results, while promising, point to many more questions about the underlying brain-behavior relationships that might explain why nonlinear EEG measures are useful digital biomarkers of ASD.

Methods

Participants

Participants were infants enrolled in an IRB-approved collaborative longitudinal study conducted at Boston Children’s Hospital/Harvard Medical School and Boston University. All components of the study were approved by the IRB review boards at both institutions and are covered under IRB guidelines approved by both institutions. Written, informed consent was provided by the parents or guardians prior to their child’s participation in the study.

Participants are classified into one of three outcome groups based on their original family recruitment group and the clinical determination of whether the child had a positive or negative diagnosis for ASD at the end of the study. Infants in the low risk controls (LRC) family recruitment group had at least one typically developing older sibling and no first-degree relatives with a known developmental disorder, based on a screening questionnaire. Infants recruited into the high risk for ASD (HRA) recruitment group had an older sibling with an ASD diagnosis (not due to a known genetic disorder; e.g. Fragile X syndrome). The older siblings all had expert clinical community diagnoses, which were confirmed by a member of the study staff using the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) 23 or the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-2) 24 . Once enrolled, infants were scheduled for visits from 3 to 36 months of age. Children who completed at least two of the visits (scheduled at 3, 6, 9, 12, 18, 24, 36 months) and completed one visit at 18 months or later, during which they were given a clinical evaluation for ASD, were included in this study. Most infants completed the final 36-month evaluation for ASD.

There were 3 outcome groups: LRC−: infants from the low risk recruitment group who did not receive an ASD diagnosis; HRA−: infants from the high-risk recruitment group who did not receive an ASD diagnosis; ASD: infants from either recruitment group who received an ASD diagnosis. Table 1 shows the number of participants in each of the family recruitment groups, their distribution into outcome groups, and the number of participants at each age. Note that not all participants provided data at every scheduled visit. The distribution of ages for the latest clinical diagnosis is also illustrated in Table 1.

figure 2

The training set is used to find the model or separating hyperplane that divides the feature space into halves. The SVM method is a formal algorithm for finding the plane that best separates the two groups. The plane that separates two groups is illustrated in Fig. 2. We note that the axes in this illustration represent the nonlinear EEG features. In practice, there are as many dimensions as there are features. The training set of data containing data points for each outcome group is used to define the plane. New data points can then be classified using their EEG features to determine the location in feature space, which lies on one side of the plane or the other.

Ideally, the training set is a large set used to find the model plane and the test set is an entirely new set of subjects that are classified to find sensitivity and specificity of the model. When such large independent data sets are not available, cross validation is a process whereby a single subject is left out of the training set, and the resulting plane is then used to classify the left-out subject. This process is repeated for every subject. For our data, ASD and LRC− subjects are used for the training set with a leave-one-out cross validation scheme. The HRA− subjects are also classified, using all the ASD and LRC− subjects for the training set. Thus, the outcome of every subject is predicted from data derived from the other subjects of the same age group. This prediction can then be compared to the known outcome and determined to be true positive, true negative, false positive, or false negative. Sensitivity, specificity and PPV are computed from these quantities with standard definitions.

The distance of a subject from the hyperplane can be used to estimate severity. This distance was scaled to approximate the Calibrated Severity Score (CSS), where 1.0 is the lowest score for children with no autism symptoms and 10 is the highest score for the most severe autism cases. Prediction of CSS values used the same training sets and leave-one-out cross validation as was used for the classification of binary outcomes.

Significance Testing

The significance of the classification results for each method was estimated empirically using the permutation approach described in 35 . Because classification accuracies were high, the empirical p-value in every case was 0, or p < .01 since 100 trials were used.

Multiscale and Frequency Bands

Multiscale entropy was first described by Costa et al. 27 as a method for analyzing physiological signals. The scaling procedure generally uses the coarse-graining or averaging algorithm illustrated in Fig. 3. For power-of-two scales (2, 4, 8, 16, …) the time series produced by this procedure are identical to the Haar wavelet transform 26 . Multiscale analysis is applied to all of the measures computed in this paper, including sample entropy, which is the primary measure for multiscale entropy.

figure 3

The connection between the coarse-graining algorithm and wavelets is important because a great deal is known about the properties of wavelet transforms. In particular, wavelets can be associated with common frequency bands. For example, if an EEG signal has a sampling rate of 256 Hz, then the signal contains frequencies up to 128 Hz, as determined by the standard Nyquist limit 36 . Scale 2 in the coarse-graining procedure is equivalent to wavelet approximation level 1, and contains all frequencies up to one-half of the original signal, 0 to 64 Hz. The averaging process removed the highest frequencies. Continuing the averaging process gives the results shown in Table 4, which shows the relationship between coarse-graining scales and frequency bands. The approximate frequencies used in neurophysiology, delta (0–4 Hz), theta (4–7 Hz) and so on, are also shown in Table 4. We note that the signals recorded in our study were hardware bandpass filtered in the 0.1–100 Hz range. Thus, the band labeled high gamma has signals in the 64–100 Hz range, and the delta band has signals in the 0.1–4 Hz range.

figure 4

Predicted LRC− CSS scores are quite low, with narrow confidence intervals, as are the assessed CSS values. Similarly, the predicted ASD scores average close to 5, similar to the actual CSS values. Predicted scores computed at all ages for both LRC− and ASD at are close to the actual measured scores.

The predicted HRA− scores are intermediate between the LRC− and ASD scores, and considerably higher than the actual CSS values for the HRA− group. Correlation coefficients are strong for predictions at all ages, though we note, as in the previous analysis, a decline at age 12.

In summary, the distance from the model classification hyperplane enabled an estimated ADOS calibrated severity score to be computed from EEG features alone as early as 3 months of age. The estimated scores were strongly correlated with the actual 36-month CSS values.

Significant Group Differences

Significant differences between the ASD and LRC− groups were found in several sensors, frequency bands, and nonlinear measures, as shown in Figs 5–7. These plots display differences between group values using color for every nonlinear value, at every sensor location (horizontal axis of subplots) and every frequency band (vertical axis on subplots). Red colors indicate that the values for this feature are higher in the ASD group. Blue colors indicate that ASD values are lower for that feature. Color saturation (darker red or darker blue) is correlated to the significance of the group differences. The scale on the right of the figure shows p-values of the color saturation. White areas with washed-out color indicate p-values close to 1.0, hence no significant differences.

figure 5

figure 6

figure 7

The plots can be interpreted as follows: The vertical axis for each subplot represents six frequency bands, from low (delta) to high (gamma+), as defined in Table 4. The horizontal axis for each subplot is the scalp location. The axis labels are shown in a single large horizontal label across the bottom of the plot. The left or right side of each subplot corresponds to left or right sensors, respectively. Centrally located sensor values are in the center of the subplots.

Developmental Trends in Selected Features

Several scientific questions that emerge from this work concern the fundamental neurophysiological explanations for the nonlinear features, sensors, and frequencies that are correlated with or predictive of ASD. Detailed answers are beyond the scope of this research. It is hoped that this study will encourage other researchers to contribute to this approach to help further develop the neurodevelopmental applications of nonlinear EEG analysis.

Visual inspection of Figs 5–7 reveals that several nonlinear values and sensor regions appear to exhibit significant differences among the three groups. First, we note that several features derived from RQA showed similar developmental trends (DET, L_max, RR) and thus may contain redundant information. DET may be representative of these values. Other features showed trends that did not appear to differentiate the three groups. Sample entropy appeared to follow a different trend than the RQA features and thus may represent independent information from the other measures. Thus, as an initial examination of developmental trends in these nonlinear features, SampE and DET were selected for closer review. Trends in these values in selected brain regions are shown in growth plots in Figs 8–12 and discussed briefly.

figure 8

figure 9

figure 10

figure 11

figure 12

Research on physiological signals has consistently revealed that lower entropy over many scales or frequency bands is often associated with pathological conditions 27,37,38,39 . Higher determinism (DET) in EEG signals was found to be associated with absence epilepsy and autism 20 . Sample Entropy and DET represent different signal qualities that are not fully understood in the context of neurophysiology, but, as discussed above, appear to be independent of each other. We emphasize again that the this is only an initial evaluation of the many features that have been used in this study and further research will be required to fully explore the neurophysiological meaning of nonlinear EEG analysis.

Developmental trends in either SampE or DET are now reviewed in one of five brain regions that appear to be relevant to ASD. The plot lines shown in Figs 8–12 are derived from the value of the nonlinear feature, either SampE or DET, by averaging all the values at the sensor(s) indicated, and averaging over all the indicated frequencies. Plots for each sensor (19 of them), every frequency band (6), and every nonlinear value (9) may be revealing in future studies, but is well beyond the scope of this paper.

Left temporal

The T7 sensor reveals that SampE in higher frequencies (beta and gamma) follows a different trajectory in the LRC− group than in the other groups. From 3–9 months, HRA− and ASD groups are similar, and different from the LRC− group. These are seen in Fig. 8. Thereafter, from 9 to 36 months, HRA− and LRC− are very close, while the ASD group first decreases from 9 to 18 months, then increases and crosses the other two trajectories.

Right temporal-parietal

Figure 9 shows changes in sample entropy in the right temporal-parietal region (sensors T8, P4, P8). Frequencies in the theta, alpha, beta, and gamma bands were found to have similar trends and magnitudes, so are considered as a whole here. The sample entropy is similar in all three groups until 6 months, but then diverges significantly by 9 months and the groups continue to diverge through 36 months, maintaining consistent and significant differences. The ASD and LRC− groups are the farthest apart, with the HRA− group having values intermediate between the higher LRC− and the lower ASD entropy values. We note that the HRA− group appears to follow the ASD trend from 9 to 12 months, but then stops the downward trend of the ASD group and tends to normalize back toward the LRC− group, similar to the trajectory in T7 as seen in Fig. 8.

Left frontal-temporal (F7)

We see in Fig. 10 that DET in frequencies theta through gamma is consistently lower in the LRC− group, distinct from both the HRA− and ASD groups, as early as 3 months. The trajectories of the three groups converge at 12–18 months, then diverge again. The intermediate HRA− group appears to follow the ASD group more closely than the LRC− group. These findings suggest that classification at early ages (3–9 months) would be better using features from this region, with less discriminatory ability around 12 months of age.

Frontal

As shown in Fig. 11, DET in the entire frontal region (F7, F3, Fp1, Fp2, F4, F8) across frequency bands theta-alpha-beta-gamma are not significantly different until after 15–18 months of age, after which the values for the LRC− group drop significantly away from the other two groups. Lower DET is generally associated with healthy neural signals 20 . Values for the ASD group continues to rise moderately from 9 to 36 months. The HRA− group remains intermediate between the LRC− and ASD groups consistently after about 12 months. This seems to be consistent with the later development of the frontal region, where differentiation between groups is not apparent until after 12 months.

Posterior region

SampE of the delta frequency band in the posterior region (O1, O2 sensors) is similar across all three groups until 9 months of age, when the ASD group diverges from the LRC− and HRA− groups, which follow similar trajectories. The sample entropy is lower in the ASD group throughout. Integrated EEG and eye-tracking studies found atypicalities in occipital delta rhythms in children with ASD 40 . The authors suggest that development in this brain region is associated with joint attention. Joint attention begins to emerge in typical infants at about 9 months and is fully developed by 18 months 41 . The consistently lower sample entropy seen in Fig. 12 may be related to underdevelopment of joint gaze in infants later diagnosed with ASD.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to demonstrate that nonlinear values computed from relatively short segments of resting state EEG signals contain information that may be used as biomarker profiles to enable an early prediction of a future outcome of ASD. The finding that several measures of signal complexity, taken from multiple brain regions, rather than a single variable or biological parameter, is consistent with the view of ASD as an “an emergent disorder that is characterized by the loss of social communication skills in the period between 9 and 24 months … defined on the basis of alterations in the developmental trajectories across multiple domains” 42 .

Behavioral markers of ASD, such as differences in social engagement, have not yet been identified within the first year of life. It has been proposed that ASD emerges only after a typical developmental trajectory in the first 12 months becomes atypical in later infancy and toddlerhood 43 . Moreover, “these core developmental constructs appear to be more sensitive to risk status (i.e. high risk versus low risk) rather than ASD outcome” 43 . Alternatively, it may be that atypical behaviors associated with core ASD symptoms are not observed in early infancy. In either case, it is possible that subtle behavioral characteristics might introduce artifacts into the EEG signals, particularly in frontal regions due to facial muscles or eye movements. However, since efforts to explicitly measure these in young infants have so far failed, it is unlikely that artifacts could completely account for the results presented here.

A recent study using data from the same infants used in the present manuscript found that reduced frontal high-alpha power at 3 months was associated with reduced expressive language skills at 12 months, but did not predict ASD-specific outcomes. No evidence that this association persists over the second and third years of life was found 17 . An earlier study with this data examined the relationship of spectral power to outcome 16 . While group differences were found, they were not predictive of outcome. A review of EEG analysis methods for detecting ASD risk concluded that “current EEG signal analysis is not able to identify children with ASD with sufficient sensitivity or specificity to be clinically useful at this time” 44 . The methods reviewed included spectral power analysis (21 studies), functional connectivity by correlation or synchronization (12 studies), and information dynamics, which includes nonlinear methods (7 studies). Nonlinear methods were the least studied EEG analysis method, yet showed promise as an early biomarker for ASD 33,37 , and many later studies. A review of EEG methods for mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) detection also found that spectral power measures were not adequate as a biomarker, yet nonlinear methods were described as having as yet “unknown, but high perceived potential” 45 .

The motivation for selecting the signal features was simply to use a broad range of features that might be used for functional characterization of a complex system through time series analysis 36,46,47 . Power in each frequency band was computed, but feature-ranking algorithms did not select power as a contributor to the predictions, so it was not included here. The list of features used here is unlikely to be complete, as new approaches will continue to be developed. However, multiscale analysis of entropy, DFA, and the several recurrence quantitative analysis (RQA) values are a reasonably complete list at this time. RQA is a relatively new nonlinear analysis approach that purports to give a complete characterization of nonlinear dynamical systems and might be sufficient alone 32,48 . This remains to be evaluated. RQA contains measures of entropy, a measure related to the Lyapunov exponent, and several others. In future research, we will work to optimize this list and identify the most diagnostically useful measures, as well as the most useful sensor locations and scales or frequency bands. But this will be a very large optimization problem that will likely require much larger datasets and a concerted effort by the cognitive neuroscience community together with data scientists.

It is not known how many EEG sensors, or what placement, is optimal for an effective biomarker profile to detect ASD. The 64 or 128 sensor nets chosen for this study were found to be relatively easy to place on young infants, and were well-tolerated by participants. For this study, a 19-sensor subset was initially used. From a purely computational perspective, even using 19 sensors results in a large feature set, as discussed previously and illustrated in Figs 5–7. Some studies have suggested that 19 sensors might be sufficient to detect ASD 20,49 . Thus, nineteen sensors uniformly distributed across the scalp using the standard 10–20 montage were chosen in this study as the minimum necessary to sample most of the scalp. Because predictive results were excellent with this set of sensors, there was no need to incorporate more than our initially chosen 19 sensors. Future studies may explore whether fewer sensors are adequate, or whether incorporating more sensors, perhaps focused in certain regions, would enable greater accuracy, or detection of a variety of ASD subtypes. A practical protocol for incorporating EEG measurements into primary care will have to consider issues such as cost, ease of use, and tolerance by infants and children of all ages, but these questions are beyond the scope of this study.

Although the number of infants available for the 3 month assessments is approximately one-third the number at other age groups (41 total: 11 ASD, 14 LRC−, 16 HRA), the size of the data set is still sufficient to draw meaningful conclusions. A recent study used 3-month data for analysis and found significant results 17 . The results found at 3 months are consistent with those at 6 months, and the trend from 3–6–9 and so on, is consistent. This at least suggests that significant and potentially useful information can be found at 3 months of age. Taken together, the 3-month data does not detract from results and conclusions from the other age groups, and adds support to the hypothesis that neural correlates of a later diagnosis of ASD may be found before 6 months of age, possibly 3 months or even earlier.

Although the EEG measurements used in this study are different from the functional MRI measures used in the recent study by 12 , the machine learning methodology used to predict ASD outcomes in the first year of life is similar in both studies, and relatively standard. Our approach to analyzing EEG as a set of time series produced by a complex system yields similar predictive results. Moreover, our results suggest that measurable brain activity as early as 3 months of age may be sufficient to predict the severity of ASD symptoms years later.

Recently, performance-based measures such as atypical eye gaze patterns 50,51,52 , unusual vocalization 51 , or fixation on geometric patterns 53 have highlighted the many potential early markers of ASD risk. Many brain recording (ERP) and imaging techniques such as DTI 54,55 and sleep fMRI 53 have shown great promise to identify brain correlates of prodromal ASD, but may not be practical for routine testing of normal infants due to their cost and difficulty. The significant EEG feature differences found in our study may be neural correlates of these behavioral measures and is an area for future exploration. For example, the quantitative results in Figs 5–7 might be correlated with specific behavioral measures.

Significant differences were found in a number of EEG features, as shown graphically in Figs 5–7. Visual inspection reveals that a subtle but significant change occurs at about 12 months. More focused examination of changes in specific regions in sample entropy and determinism reveal that the convergence in the three months before and after 12 months is consistent across most regions, features, and frequency ranges. This can also be seen in the regional trajectories in Figs 8–12.

The neural interpretation of nonlinear electrophysiological results presented here require further extensive research, analogous perhaps to the micro- and macro- neural structural interpretation of Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) indices that are still subject to ongoing research 56 . Nevertheless, some general interpretations are suggested by our results. Atypical development in the frontal and temporal lobes are believed to be involved in ASD 56,57 . The left frontal (Fig. 10) and total bilateral frontal (Fig. 11) trajectories appear to support this. Divergence of ASD in both of these regions appears after 18 months of age. Since frontal regions develop later than posterior regions, these may not be as useful for early detection of ASD.

Atypical lateralization of language areas has been found to be common in people with ASD 58,59 , including reduced structural asymmetry in fronto-temporal language regions, with more atypical asymmetries linked to more substantive language impairment 60 . Typical Sylvian Fissure (SF) asymmetry has been associated with typical anterior and posterior parietal asymmetry 59 . Finch et al. 61 found differences in lateralization patterns of ERPs to speech stimuli across the same groups as the present study by 12 months of age, with the ASD group showing reversed lateralization compared to the LRC− group 61 . Taken together, significant differences between ASD and non-ASD subjects may be present as early as 12 months or before in temporal, frontal and temporal-parietal regions.

The differing trajectories in left temporal (T7) region for our three groups, as shown in Fig. 8, may reflect differences in a critical auditory process that develops during this time. We speculate that the deviation of the ASD development in this region from the LRC− group is related to auditory processing.

Figure 9 shows the HRA− and ASD groups following parallel paths until 9 months, significantly different from the LRC− group, in the right temporal-parietal region. Starting at 12 months, the HRA− curve breaks from the ASD trajectory and begins to parallel the LRC− group, though at an intermediate level. Both EEG and MEG studies show atypical brain activity in children with autism in primary and association auditory cortices 62 . This atypical activity may be reflected by the significantly different entropy levels found in the right temporal-parietal region after 6 months.

An apparent shift in the EEG-derived values at 12 months can be seen in the overall feature maps of Figs 5–7 as well as in some of the regional curves shown in Figs 8–12. This 12-month shift may correspond to the drop accuracy of HRA− classification as that occurs around this age. In Table 5(b), HRA− prediction is >95% for 3 and 6 month infants, then drops to between 63% and 71% thereafter (79% if 6 and 9 month values are combined). Also, the numbers of uncertain participants that are too close to the dividing plane increases considerably in the 9 to 12 month range and remains high thereafter.

One of the future challenges with our analysis is to create developmental high-dimensional trajectories that cannot be visualized in three dimensions. The trajectories in Figs 8–12 show distinct changes and differences between the three groups that may be indicators of atypical developmental paths in those regions that are associated with the later emergence of autistic characteristics.

Predicted Severity Scores

The relatively strong correlation between predicted and actual measured CSS scores suggests that the EEG analysis presented herein may be useful not only as a means of predicting a future diagnosis of ASD, but also for assessing the severity of future symptoms. We note that a number of participants labeled as ASD had ADOS summary scores of 1 or 2, which and some non-ASD participants in the HRA− group had ADOS scores of 3 to 5. The correlations are reduced considerably by these seemingly inconsistent or heterogeneous summary scores. The predicted summary scores for the HRA− group are somewhat higher than the actual scores, which are only slightly higher than the LRC− group. It is not known which scores might be more closely indicative of actual real-world behaviors. Importantly, the predicted summary scores predict the actual group membership quite well, with relatively narrow confidence intervals around the mean.

Research suggests that features of ASD are not restricted to individuals who are diagnosed with ASD, and that there is pronounced variation within the general population relating to ASD traits, which reflect similar (though less severe) social-cognitive and behavioral features to those observed in ASDs 63 . Cognitive tests in another study revealed similarities between children with ASD and non-ASD siblings of children with ASD on standard intelligence tests, suggesting that the common cognitive profile could be an intermediate phenotype of this syndrome 64 . Studies of older children have found that siblings and parents are more likely to show mild impairments in language 65,66 , non-verbal communication (Ruser et al. 66 ), theory of mind 67,68 and face processing compared to controls 68 . These findings about the heterogeneity of ASD in the general population suggest that our results regarding the CSS scores might be expanded to include more refined subtypes of ASD.

The results in Table 5 warrant further examination. When training a classifier to recognize EEG features that predict a specific outcome, the training sets must be distinct. Because ASD occurs along a spectrum, training a classifier to make a binary decision (ASD or not) with subjects that have essentially the full range of ASD characteristics leads to problems with the very definition of ASD. Subjects that have ADOS summary scores that put them near the borderline of “mild ASD” versus “not ASD, but exhibiting ASD-like characteristics” requires the EEG classifier to make decisions that result in for which even human clinicians have low inter-rater agreement. This is exactly what we see in Table 5.b. The age trend is interesting as well: at 3 and 6 months, even the HRA− subjects are easily classified “correctly” as not-ASD. This may simply be because ASD brain function has not yet fully emerged. After 9 months, the HRA− subjects are more difficult to classify, perhaps because ASD-like brain function, associated with CSS summary scores that approach the borderline, is emerging.

This situation is remedied in Table 5.c where we allow 3 classification outcomes: ASD, not-ASD, and ‘uncertain’. The latter category occurs when the EEG features lead to a score that puts the subject very near the classification plane, as illustrated in Fig. 2. Using this approach, 116 subjects with data at 6 and 9 months were classified perfectly, with only 7 of the 116 labeled as “uncertain”. It may be that a longitudinal risk assessment model that updates with each new measurement might enable these uncertainties to be resolved as the child passes 12 months, or 18 months. This remains an area for future research.

Examining the results in Table 5.c, it appears that the lowest predictive accuracy occurs at 12 months. Also, the largest number of infants labeled as “uncertain” occurs at 9, 12, and 18 months. This seems to correlate with the shift in EEG feature values that occurs at around 12 months. By 36 months, the number of uncertain labels has decreased and the predictive accuracy has improved again. However, the predictive accuracy appears to be somewhat lower at 36 months. In Figs 8–12 it is quite apparent that the confidence intervals become much wider at 36 months, suggesting that all groups are becoming more diverse in their electrophysiological activity.

The greatest remaining challenge in the evaluation of nonlinear signal analysis methods is to discover the neural and behavioral meaning of the various EEG measures. Terms used to label the features used in this study are derived from physical systems. Entropy, determinism, laminarity, and so on have meaning in the context of turbulent fluid flow, for example. The RQA-derived values appeared to be similar, and distinctly different from SampE and DFA. Thus, regional plots (Figs 8–12) examined only SampE and DET. Further research is required to determine if more information, perhaps subtle, can be found in the array of RQA, or other nonlinear, signal features. Translating these mathematical constructs to networks of millions of neural generators cannot be done through analytical mathematics alone, but may require many empirical studies whereby differences in entropy, for example, are compared to many cognitive and behavioral phenotypes. Machine learning algorithms are useful for recognizing that significant correlations between patterns of features and outcomes exist, but these are unable to interpret these correlations in electrophysiological terms. One of the goals of this study is to encourage the broader neuroscience community to become involved in this research and contribute to a better and deeper understanding of the relationship between nonlinear (and multiscale) analysis of electrophysiological signals and their meaning in the context of brain-behavior relationships.

Conclusions

The results presented in this paper are consistent with and greatly extend our previous study with a subset from this cohort of infants (Bosl et al., 2011). Nonlinear analysis of EEG signals extracts information that is significantly different in children who develop ASD, as early as 3 months of age. Predictions of the diagnostic outcomes were highly accurate using measurements as early as 3 months of age. Our analytic approach was not only associated with the binary outcome, but also strongly correlated with symptom severity as measured by CSS scores. Developmental trajectories of SampE and DET in key brain regions associated with ASD revealed significant differences between the three groups. In general, the ASD group diverged from the LRC− group at early ages in the left temporal and right temporal-parietal regions, and diverged later, after 18 months, in frontal regions. Predicted severity scores were significantly correlated with actual scores using EEG measurements taken at 3 months of age and older. This suggests that EEG measurement using the method presented here is a promising technology for monitoring neural development in a broad population of children. Future research with larger and more diverse populations is needed to determine the clinical applicability of this approach to ASD detection in general populations. As our results when combining 6- and 9-month suggest, longitudinal studies that create trajectories rather than point measurements at a single age might yield further insights.

References

  1. Baio, J. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2010. MMWR Surveill Summ63, 1–21, http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/ss6302a1.htm?s_cid = ss6302a1_w (2014).
  2. Amaral, D. G. et al. In pursuit of neurophenotypes: The consequences of having autism and a big brain. Autism Res10, 711–722, https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1755 (2017). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Ozonoff, S. et al. A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry49(256–266), e251–252 doi:00004583-201003000-00009 (2010). Google Scholar
  4. Reiersen, A. M. Early Identification of Autism Spectrum Disorder: Is Diagnosis by Age 3 a Reasonable Goal? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry56, 284–285, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.02.003 (2017). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Sheldrick, R. C. & Garfinkel, D. Is a Positive Developmental-Behavioral Screening Score Sufficient to Justify Referral? A Review of Evidence and Theory. Acad Pediatr17, 464–470, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2017.01.016 (2017). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Steiner, A. M., Goldsmith, T. R., Snow, A. V. & Chawarska, K. Practitioner’s guide to assessment of autism spectrum disorders in infants and toddlers. J Autism Dev Disord42, 1183–1196, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1376-9 (2012). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Pettersson, E., Anckarsater, H., Gillberg, C. & Lichtenstein, P. Different neurodevelopmental symptoms have a common genetic etiology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry54, 1356–1365, https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12113 (2013). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Gliga, T., Jones, E. J., Bedford, R., Charman, T. & Johnson, M. H. From early markers to neuro-developmental mechanisms of autism. Dev Rev34, 189–207, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.05.003 (2014). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  9. Johnson, M. H., Jones, E. J. & Gliga, T. Brain adaptation and alternative developmental trajectories. Dev Psychopathol27, 425–442, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000073 (2015). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Hernandez, L. M., Rudie, J. D., Green, S. A., Bookheimer, S. & Dapretto, M. Neural signatures of autism spectrum disorders: insights into brain network dynamics. Neuropsychopharmacology: official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology40, 171–189, https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2014.172 (2015). ArticleGoogle Scholar
  11. Kana, R. K., Libero, L. E. & Moore, M. S. Disrupted cortical connectivity theory as an explanatory model for autism spectrum disorders. Phys Life Rev8, 410–437, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2011.10.001 (2011). ArticleADSPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Emerson, R. W. et al. Functional neuroimaging of high-risk 6-month-old infants predicts a diagnosis of autism at 24 months of age. Sci Transl Med9, https://doi.org/10.1126/scitranslmed.aag2882 (2017).
  13. Chen, C. P. et al. Diagnostic classification of intrinsic functional connectivity highlights somatosensory, default mode, and visual regions in autism. Neuroimage Clin8, 238–245, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2015.04.002 (2015). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Elison, J. T. et al. Frontolimbic neural circuitry at 6 months predicts individual differences in joint attention at 9 months. Dev Sci16, 186–197, https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12015 (2013). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Elison, J. T. et al. White matter microstructure and atypical visual orienting in 7-month-olds at risk for autism. The American journal of psychiatry170, 899–908, https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12091150 (2013). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Tierney, A. L., Gabard-Durnam, L., Vogel-Farley, V., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Nelson, C. A. Developmental trajectories of resting EEG power: an endophenotype of autism spectrum disorder. PLoS One7, e39127, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0039127 (2012). ArticleADSCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Levin, A. R., Varcin, K. J., O’Leary, H. M., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Nelson, C. A. EEG Power at 3 Months in Infants at High Familial Risk for Autism. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders9, 1–13 (2017). ArticleGoogle Scholar
  18. Gao, Z. & Jin, N. Complex network from time series based on phase space reconstruction. Chaos19, 033137, https://doi.org/10.1063/1.3227736 (2009). ArticleADSPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Bosl, W. In Neurophenotypes: Advancing Psychiatry and Neuropsychology in the “OMICS” Era Innovations in Cognitive Neuroscience (eds Vinoth Jagaroo & Susan Santangelo) (Springer, 2016).
  20. Bosl, W. J., Loddenkemper, T. & Nelson, C. A. Nonlinear EEG Analysis Reveals Commonalities and Differences in Autism and Absence Epilepsy. Neuropsychiatric Electrophysiology3, https://doi.org/10.1186/s40810-017-0023-x (2017).
  21. David Hairston, W. et al. Usability of four commercially-oriented EEG systems. J Neural Eng11, 046018, https://doi.org/10.1088/1741-2560/11/4/046018 (2014). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Maskeliunas, R., Damasevicius, R., Martisius, I. & Vasiljevas, M. Consumer-grade EEG devices: are they usable for control tasks? PeerJ4, e1746, https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1746 (2016). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  23. Rutter, M., Bailey, A. & Lord, C. In Western Psychological Services (2003).
  24. Lord, C. et al. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord30, 205–223 (2000). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Gotham, K., Pickles, A. & Lord, C. Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord39, 693–705, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-008-0674-3 (2009). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Walker, J. S. A primer on wavelets and their scientific applications. 2nd edn, (Chapman & Hall/CRC, 2008).
  27. Costa, M., Goldberger, A. L. & Peng, C. K. Multiscale entropy analysis of biological signals. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys71, 021906 (2005). ArticleADSMathSciNetPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Bosl, W. J., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Nelson, C. A. EEG Complexity as a Biomarker for Autism Spectrum Disorder. BMC medicine9 (2011).
  29. Bhyri, C., Hamde, S. T. & Waghmare, L. M. ECG feature extraction and disease diagnosis. J Med Eng Technol35, 354–361, https://doi.org/10.3109/03091902.2011.595530 (2011). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Webber, C. L. & Zbilut, J. P. In Tutorials in contemporary nonlinear methods for the behavioral sciences (eds M. A. Riley & G. Van Orden) (National Science Foundation, 2005).
  31. Marwan, N., Recurrence Plots and Cross Recurrence Plots. http://www.recurrence-plot.tk, (2012).
  32. Marwan, N., Romano, M. C., Thiel, M. & Kurths, J. Recurrence plots for the analysis of complex systems. Physics Reports438, 237–329 (2007). ArticleADSMathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  33. Bosl, W., Tierney, A., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Nelson, C. EEG complexity as a biomarker for autism spectrum disorder risk. BMC medicine9, https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-9-18 (2011).
  34. Guyon, I., Weston, J., Barnhill, S. & Vapnik, V. Gene selection for cancer classification using support vector machines. Machine Learning46, 389–422 (2002). ArticleMATHGoogle Scholar
  35. Golland, P. & Fischl, B. Permutation tests for classification: towards statistical significance in image-based studies. Inf Process Med Imaging18, 330–341 (2003). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Drongelen, W. v. Signal processing for neuroscientists: introduction to the analysis of physiological signals. (Academic Press, 2007).
  37. Catarino, A., Churches, O., Baron-Cohen, S., Andrade, A. & Ring, H. Atypical EEG complexity in autism spectrum conditions: a multiscale entropy analysis. Clin Neurophysiol122, 2375–2383, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2011.05.004 (2011). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Takahashi, T. et al. Antipsychotics reverse abnormal EEG complexity in drug-naive schizophrenia: a multiscale entropy analysis. Neuroimage51, 173–182, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.009 (2010). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  39. Vandendriessche, B. et al. A multiscale entropy-based tool for scoring severity of systemic inflammation. Crit Care Med42, e560–569, https://doi.org/10.1097/CCM.0000000000000299 (2014). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Billeci, L. et al. An integrated EEG and eye-tracking approach for the study of responding and initiating joint attention in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Sci Rep7, 13560, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-13053-4 (2017). ArticleADSPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  41. Bruinsma, Y., Koegel, R. L. & Koegel, L. K. Joint attention and children with autism: a review of the literature. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev10, 169–175, https://doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.20036 (2004). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Tager-Flusberg, H. The origins of social impairments in autism spectrum disorder: Studies of infants at risk. Neural NetworksAug6, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2010.07.008 (2010).
  43. Varcin, K. J. & Jeste, S. S. The emergence of autism spectrum disorder: insights gained from studies of brain and behaviour in high-risk infants. Curr Opin Psychiatry30, 85–91, https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000312 (2017). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Gurau, O., Bosl, W. J. & Newton, C. R. How Useful Is Electroencephalography in the Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorders and the Delineation of Subtypes: A Systematic Review. Front Psychiatry8, 121, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00121 (2017). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  45. Rapp, P. E. et al. Traumatic brain injury detection using electrophysiological methods. Frontiers in human neuroscience9, 11, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00011 (2015). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  46. Kantz, H. & Schreiber, T. Nonlinear time series analysis. 2nd edn, (Cambridge University Press, 2004).
  47. Sörnmo, L. & Laguna, P. Bioelectrical signal processing in cardiac and neurological applications. (Elsevier Academic Press, 2005).
  48. Webber, C. L. & Marwan, N. In Understanding Complex Systems (Springer, New York, 2015).
  49. Heunis, T. M., Aldrich, C. & de Vries, P. J. Recent Advances in Resting-State Electroencephalography Biomarkers for Autism Spectrum Disorder-A Review of Methodological and Clinical Challenges. Pediatr Neurol61, 28–37, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2016.03.010 (2016). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Bedford, R. et al. Precursors to social and communication difficulties in infants at-risk for autism: gaze following and attentional engagement. J Autism Dev Disord42, 2208–2218, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1450-y (2012). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Jones, W. & Klin, A. Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2-6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature504, 427–431, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12715 (2013). ArticleADSCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  52. Wagner, J. B., Hirsch, S. B., Vogel-Farley, V. K., Redcay, E. & Nelson, C. A. Eye-tracking, autonomic, and electrophysiological correlates of emotional face processing in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord43, 188–199, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1565-1 (2013). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  53. Pierce, K., Conant, D., Hazin, R., Stoner, R. & Desmond, J. Preference for geometric patterns early in life as a risk factor for autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry68, 101–109, https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.113 (2011). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Elsabbagh, M. et al. The development of face orienting mechanisms in infants at-risk for autism. Behav Brain Res251, 147–154, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.07.030 (2012). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Elsabbagh, M. et al. Infant neural sensitivity to dynamic eye gaze is associated with later emerging autism. Curr Biol22, 338–342, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.056 (2012). ArticleCASPubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  56. Hoppenbrouwers, M., Vandermosten, M. & Boet, B. Autism as a disconnection syndrome: A qualitative and quantitative review of diffusion tensor imaging studies. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders8, 387–412 (2013). ArticleGoogle Scholar
  57. Geschwind, D. H. & Levitt, P. Autism spectrum disorders: developmental disconnection syndromes. Curr Opin Neurobiol17, 103–111, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2007.01.009 (2007). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Knaus, T. A. et al. Language laterality in autism spectrum disorder and typical controls: a functional, volumetric, and diffusion tensor MRI study. Brain Lang112, 113–120, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2009.11.005 (2010). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Knaus, T. A., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Foundas, A. L. Sylvian fissure and parietal anatomy in children with autism spectrum disorder. Behav Neurol25, 327–339, https://doi.org/10.3233/BEN-2012-110214 (2012). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  60. Lindell, A. K. & Hudry, K. Atypicalities in cortical structure, handedness, and functional lateralization for language in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychol Rev23, 257–270, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-013-9234-5 (2013). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Finch, K. H., Seery, A. M., Talbott, M. R., Nelson, C. A. & Tager-Flusberg, H. Lateralization of ERPs to speech and handedness in the early development of Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Neurodev Disord9, 4, https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-017-9185-x (2017). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  62. Marco, E. J., Hinkley, L. B., Hill, S. S. & Nagarajan, S. S. Sensory processing in autism: a review of neurophysiologic findings. Pediatric research69, 48R–54R, https://doi.org/10.1203/PDR.0b013e3182130c54 (2011). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  63. Gokcen, E., Petrides, K. V., Hudry, K., Frederickson, N. & Smillie, L. D. Sub-threshold autism traits: the role of trait emotional intelligence and cognitive flexibility. Br J Psychol105, 187–199, https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12033 (2014). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Gizzonio, V., Avanzini, P., Fabbri-Destro, M., Campi, C. & Rizzolatti, G. Cognitive abilities in siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. Exp Brain Res232, 2381–2390, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-3935-8 (2014). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Lindgren, K. A., Folstein, S. E., Tomblin, J. B. & Tager-Flusberg, H. Language and reading abilities of children with autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment and their first-degree relatives. Autism Res2, 22–38, https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.63 (2009). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  66. Ruser, T. F. et al. Communicative competence in parents of children with autism and parents of children with specific language impairment. J Autism Dev Disord37, 1323–1336, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0274-z (2007). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Baron-Cohen, S. & Hammer, J. Parents of Children with Asperger Syndrome: What is the Cognitive Phenotype? J Cogn Neurosci9, 548–554, https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.4.548 (1997). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Dawson, G. et al. Neurocognitive and electrophysiological evidence of altered face processing in parents of children with autism: implications for a model of abnormal development of social brain circuitry in autism. Dev Psychopathol17, 679–697, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050327 (2005). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Gomez, C. & Hornero, R. Entropy and Complexity Analyses in Alzheimer’s Disease: An MEG Study. Open Biomed Eng J4, 223–235, https://doi.org/10.2174/1874120701004010223TOBEJ-4-223 (2010). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  70. Goldberger, A. L. Fractal variability versus pathologic periodicity: complexity loss and stereotypy in disease. Perspect Biol Med40, 543–561 (1997). ArticleCASPubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Peng, C.-K., Costa, M. & Goldberger, A. L. Adaptive Data Analysis of Complex Fluctuations in Physiologic Time Series. Advances in Adaptive Data Analysis1, 61–70 (2009). ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
  72. Schinkel, S., Marwan, N. & Kurths, J. Brain signal analysis based on recurrences. J Physiol Paris103, 315–323, doi:S0928-4257(09)00029-110.1016/j.jphysparis.2009.05.007 (2019). ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Hirata, Y. & Aihara, K. Statistical Tests for Serial Dependence and Laminarity on Recurrence Plots. International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos, 1077–1084 (2011).

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) grant R21 MH 093753 (to WJB), National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (NIDCD) grant R21 DC08647 (to HTF), NIDCD grant R01 DC 10290 (to HTF and CAN) and a grant from the Simons Foundation (to CAN, HTF, and WJB). We are especially grateful to the staff and students who worked on the study and to the families who participated.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

  1. Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston, USA William J. Bosl & Charles A. Nelson
  2. Harvard Medical School, Boston, USA William J. Bosl & Charles A. Nelson
  3. University of San Francisco, San Francisco, USA William J. Bosl
  4. Boston University, Boston, USA Helen Tager-Flusberg
  5. Harvard Graduate School of Education, Cambridge, USA Charles A. Nelson
  1. William J. Bosl